The Darl Living Surface: A Transitional Archaic Camp
Explore Darl Component map by rolling over and clicking on zones. |
Interpretation of Drop Zone DataAnalysis of the spatial distribution of materials using an ethnographic model of drop-toss zones indicates differential discard patterns. Formal stone tools are found around the features in the drop zone and debris such as mussel shells are found in the surrounding toss zones. Stone cores and expedient flakes are scattered across the surface, but utilized flakes made on bifacial thinning flakes are significantly clustered within the drop zones. In other words, the data show clear spatial patterning, rather than random distributions, which are interpreted as the result of cultural behaviors, on the Darl occupational surface. Binford hypothesized that two types of artifacts should be found within the drop zone— those that are dropped, which are often smaller pieces such as bone splinters, and those that are set down like a coffee cup, to use his example. The set-down items are those intended for further use. In the Darl component, projectile points, trimmed flakes, and bifaces (such as knives) are interpreted as items being actively used to accomplish a task, or objects being maintained or worked on (such as points being hafted or resharpened). On a wider scale across the site living surface, the "drop-toss" zone analysis indicates spatially segregated activity areas around central loci. These broad patterns are similar to those observed in contemporary small hunter-gatherer camps with clusters of houses (huts). Lacking a more extensive view of the living surface, however, the location of possible houses could not be clearly discerned. Although additional excavations might have identified traces of such features, forager huts are often very subtle in the archeologial record and can only be inferred to have existed. Most of the evidence on the site derived from short-term, intensive food-gathering activities. Several other ethnographic studies were consulted as possible correlates for the McKinney Roughs site. A plan map of a hunter-gatherer camp in the African Kalahari Desert (shown at right) shows overall layout, including a circular cluster of activity areas with hearths, small huts, and tool kits as well as areas where trash, including bone and other materials, was discarded. Stones used for processing foods and other uses are nearby each of the house areas. There the campers processed quantities of mongongo nuts. While the five activity areas defined on the McKinney Roughs surface cannot be associated with huts or other structures, it is possible they were situated around such features at the time and evidence has not been preserved. Collectively, the activity areas and debris represent contemporaneous and probably interrelated group activities similar to the ones depicted in the figure. At McKinney Roughs, processing mussel shell appears to have been the obvious chief activity rather than mongongo nuts as in the Kalahari case. Based on cross-cultural hunter-gatherer evidence, anthropologist Polly Weissner has noted that a forager residential base often occupies about 250 square meters. To the extent that the parameters of the Darl camp surface were fully established at McKinney Roughs, the site measures approximately 12 to 15 m east to west by about 25 to 30 m north to south, or about 300 to 450 square meters and thus within the expected range of a forager residential base. Thus, although the Darl camp may have been large enough to qualify by this measure as a forager residential base camp, there is insufficient evidence to support this possibility. Summing Up the EvidenceSeveral interpretations can be made about the distribution of Darl component materials. The processing of mussel shells as a foodstuff was obviously a common site activity. Mussel shell distribution is interpreted as a toss zone. Items such as metates, hearths, and fireplaces are structural site elements abandoned or discarded where they were utilized. The distinction reveals activity areas and even specific locales where a person or person was situated. For example, in one area a metate and an associated mano were found surrounded by scattered materials, as well as a 50 cm diameter area devoid of artifacts. The area is interpreted as an occupational locus where a person, likely a woman, used the grinding stones, perhaps in the processing of mussel shell, discarded the mussel shell in a toss zone and likely left the remaining aspect of the tool kit. At McKinney Roughs, the Darl component is interpreted as a small foraging camp which contained several activity areas in roughly circular clusters. Various activity areas and possibly tool kits are evident in the patterns across a living surface that likely formed over the course of a single occupational episode. These activity areas reflect the use of space by the inhabitants of the site as they processed foodstuffs, notably mussels, foraged from the surrounding riverine environment. This view of day-to-day activities of prehistoric humans living 1000 years ago is rare among archeological sites. |
|